Emotion regulation difficulties in drug abusers

Abstract

Introduction: Various factors, including personality characteristics, are involved in addiction and perpetuation of it. Emotions and the way they are felt and responded are a part of personality that may play a role in showing tendency to drug abuse and perpetuation of this behavior. The present study aimed at investigating emotional difficulties in drug addicts. Methodology: The statistical population of this cross-sectional study included 268 individuals, of which 166 individuals were drug dependence who visited medical centers in Zahedan and Iranshahr. The remaining 120 individuals had no addiction history and were assessed with difficulty in emotion regulation scale. Sample selection was done through convenience sampling technique. Findings: Results suggested that drug-dependent people had significant difference from nondependent individuals in five aspects, namely nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulty engaging in goal-directed behaviors, difficulty in impulse control, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and lack of emotional clarity (P < 0.01). These two groups were not different only in one aspect, namely the lack of emotional awareness (P > 0.05). Evaluation of emotional difficulties and gender showed a difference between men and women in nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulty in impulse control, limited access to emotion regulation strategies (P < 0.01), and lack of emotional clarity (P < 0.05). There was no between-groups difference in difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors and the lack of emotional awareness. Conclusion: Although emotional difficulties are not the main factor in the onset and perpetuation of drug abuse, abusers suffer from them. Given the research results and those of other studies, the importance of emotion in etiology, prevention, and treatment of addiction can be highlighted.

Keywords: Difficulty in emotion regulation, drug abuse, drug dependence

How to cite this article:
Zareban I, Bakhshani NM, Bor MH, Bakhshani S. Emotion regulation difficulties in drug abusers. Ann Trop Med Public Health 2017;10:1724-8
How to cite this URL:
Zareban I, Bakhshani NM, Bor MH, Bakhshani S. Emotion regulation difficulties in drug abusers. Ann Trop Med Public Health [serial online] 2017 [cited 2020 Oct 25];10:1724-8. Available from: https://www.atmph.org/text.asp?2017/10/6/1724/222707
Introduction

In addition to the positive and constructive role of emotions in human life, they also have a destructive side. Emotions are problematic when they are expressed improperly, occur in an inappropriate context, are too intense, or last too long.[1] This dual function of emotions indicates the emotion regulation process, in which people regulate and adjust their emotion according to the situation.[2] Emotion regulation process may be automated or controlled, and conscious or subconscious.[3] Researchers have proposed different definitions for emotion regulation. According to Eisenberg and Morris,[4] emotion regulation includes the initiation, maintenance, regulation, and alteration of intensity or duration of internal emotional states, emotion-related motivations, and physical processes that often serve to achieve one’s goals. According to Cole, Martin, and Denis (2004), emotion regulation includes changes that are associated with activated emotions. These alterations include changes that occur within the emotion or other psychological processes (such as memory, attention, or social interactions). Emotion can be looked at from two perspectives: (A) emotion as a regulating factor and (B) emotion as a regulated factor (changes in activated emotions).

Emotion regulation, as a psychological variable, has been given special attention from researchers in recent decades (Golman, 1995);[5],[6] Several evidence exists on the relationship of emotion regulation with success and failure in different areas of life,[7],[8] adaptation to life stressors (Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, and Reiser, 2000), development and growth of mental disorders,[2],[9] postaccident stress,[10] generalized anxiety,[11] borderline personality disorder,[12] high-risk sexual behaviors,[13],[14] and drug abuse.[15]

Gratz and Roemer [16] identified four components of emotion regulation: (1) awareness and understanding of emotions, (2) acceptance of emotions, (3) ability to engage in goal-directed and refrain from impulsive behavior when experiencing negative emotions, and (4) the flexible use of situationally appropriate strategies to modulate emotional responses.

Awareness and understanding of emotions, as an important strategy, can be the source and cause of many psychological problems.[15] Difficulty in emotion regulation has been identified as a key element in several psychological models for different disorders including borderline personality disorder, major depression, bipolar disorder, generalized anxiety, social anxiety, eating disorder, and drug and alcohol abuse disorders.[16],[17],[18] Tull et al.[19] also in a study discovered the role of emotional difficulty in high-risk sexual behaviors and multiple sexual partners. The diagnosis of the majority of psychological disorders is based on emotional turbulence, which is fundamentally linked to defective function in emotion regulation. Cole et al.[20] believed that emotion regulation is not synonymous with emotional control and as such, does not necessarily involve immediately diminishing negative affect. It is also a defect in the attenuate of the quantity and intensity of negative feelings.[7],[20],[21] These latter approaches suggest that deficiencies in the capacity to experience (and differentiate) the full range of emotions and respond spontaneously may be just as maladaptive as deficiencies in the ability to attenuate and modulate strong negative emotions. Negative emotions are an integrated part of today’s life; therefore, emotion regulation, as one of the most important physical and psychological health factors, has undoubtedly significant potential in today’s life.[22] Given the importance of emotion regulation, this study was done to provide a better understanding of emotion regulation difficulties in the addicts by comparing them with the nonaddicts.

Methodology

This is a cross-sectional, descriptive-analytical study. The sample size included 286 individuals, among which 166 individuals were addicted, selected from those visiting addiction rehabilitation clinics in Zahedan and Iranshahr in a 4-month period. In addition, 120 individuals with no history of drug abuse were selected and evaluated, using convenience sampling technique.

Research Instrument

Difficulty in emotion regulation scale

It is a 36-item scale developed by Gratz and Roemer.[16] The measure yields a total score as well as scores on 6 subscales. These subscales are nonacceptance of emotions, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors, difficulty in impulse control, the lack of emotional awareness, limited access to emotion regulation strategies, and the lack of emotional clarity. This scale is responded based on a 5-point Likert scale. Gratz and Roemer [16] investigated the reliability and validity of this scale by applying it to 479 master’s students. This scale showed good internal consistency in the overall score (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93) and all subscales (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.8). The reliability of the test–retest in a 4–8 weeks period was reported to be appropriate (P < 0.01 for subscales and = 0.88 for the overall score). Khanzadeh et al.[23] examined the reliability and validity of difficulty in emotion regulation scale in an Iranian sample including 363 students of Shiraz University. They reported the Cronbach’s alpha of 0.86–0.88 and retest’s validity coefficient of 0.79–0.91 for these scales after 1 week.

Findings

All 286 participants were divided in two groups, namely the addicts (n = 166, 58%) and nonaddicts (n = 120, 42%). The mean age and standard deviation of drug addicts and nonaddicts were 32.65 ± 8.22 and 22 ± 5.79, respectively.

Given research findings from investigation into the subscales of difficulty in emotion regulation, it was found that the addicts gained higher scores and means than nonaddicts [Table 1].

Table 1: The t-test results from comparison of mean scores in subscales of difficulty in emotion regulation between the addicts and nonaddicts groups

Click here to view

According to the findings, there was a significant between-groups difference in nonacceptance of emotional responses, difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors, impulse control difficulties, access to emotion regulation strategies, and the lack of emotional clarity (P < 0.01). The only exception with no between-groups difference was the lack of emotional awareness. Results from the comparison of subscales of difficulties in emotion regulation between men and women are presented in [Table 2].

Table 2: The t-test results from comparison of mean scores in subscales of difficulties in emotion regulation by gender

Click here to view

There was a significant gender-difference in nonacceptance of emotional responses, impulse control difficulties, access to emotion regulation strategies, the lack of emotional clarity; whereas, no significant difference was observed in the lack of emotion awareness, and difficulties engaging in goal-directed behavior.

Discussion and Conclusion

Results from studying emotional defects and difficulties were significantly different in the addicts and nonaddicts. This indicates poor emotion regulation strategies and disability in coping with emotions and emotional management, which are consistent with the findings of Parker et al. (2008), and Goleman’s hypothesis (1995), who reported low emotional quotient in drug abusers. Nonacceptance of emotions in the addicts makes them tended to negative emotional responses while they are facing with emotions. Difficulties engaging in goal-directed behaviors assess inability to focus on goal-directed behaviors when facing with emotions. According to research results, those with stronger negative emotions have greater difficulties in displaying goal-directed behaviors, which is consistent with the findings of Gratz and Roemer 2004,[16] Tull et al.,[19] 2008, and Orgeta,[24] 2001. Difference in scores of difficulty in impulse control showed that the addicts had difficulties in impulse control and inhibition when they were facing with emotions.[25] Some researchers also indicated that difficulty in impulse control is often associated with relapse in various drugs of abuse.[26] The two groups were not different in the lack of emotional clarity, indicating that they were capable of distinguishing between emotions and motivational messages within emotions.

The strategies and approaches of emotion regulation and the lack of emotional clarity were two other investigated variables that show a between-groups difference. The use of maladaptive and inefficient strategies is related to intensity of emotions and poor understanding of them. This behavior is more common among those with more severe emotional impulses.[16] Repression, avoidance and rumination are among inefficient strategies with the maximum impact on mental disorders.[27]

The lack of clarity and poor understanding of emotions increase the sense of sadness in the person. This results in the use of maladaptive coping strategies by that person (Steven et al., 1996). Since behavior and thought are functions of emotions, any disorder and defect in emotional system can lay the ground for several disorders or be affected by them.[28]

According to Cole,[20] emotion regulation means compliance and flexibility in the use of emotion regulation strategies. Emotional adaptation includes changes in the intensity and duration of a feeling instead of emotional alteration and dissociation.[29],[30] Emotional adaptation deals with emotional experiences instead of removing and suppressing emotions. In this model, the perception of motivation decreases anxiety and increases one’s control over his/her emotions and behaviors. In addition, it leads to the control of impulses and inappropriate behaviors, and the achievement of desired goals during emotional experience (Hinshaw, 2000). In the explanation of results, two factors can be indicated: (1) drug abuse causes emotional defect, and (2) emotional defect and poor emotion management increase the probability of drug abuse. Management capability and appropriate emotion regulation strategy can contribute to healthy coping strategies (leaving the location and power of saying no) and/or unhealthy strategies (coming under pressure, consumption) when there is a risk of drug abuse. Individuals capable of positive emotion regulation are also more capable of predicting others’ demands, understanding others’ unwanted pressure, and controlling their emotions; as a result, they have higher resistance to drug abuse.[31]

Therefore, given limited access to emotion regulation strategies and difficulties engaging goal-directed behavior, emotion-based psychological interventions including dialectical behavior therapy [10] emotion-oriented therapy,[21] acceptance-based behavior therapy and mindfulness,[32] emotion regulation therapy,[33] which target emotion attenuation and defect, need extra attention.[34]

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.

References
1.
Werner K, Gross JJ. Emotion regulation and psychopatology: A conceptual framework. In: Kring AM, Gross JJ, editors. Emotion Regulation and Psychopathology: A Trans Diagnostic Approach to Etiology and Treatment. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010.
2.
Coutinho J, Rebeiro E, Ferririnha R, Dias P. The Portuguese version of the difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale and its relationship with psychopathological symptoms. Rev Psiq Clin 2010;37:145-51.
3.
Gross JJ. The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 1998;2:271-99.
4.
Eisenberg N, Morris AS. Childrens emotion-related regulation. In: Kaill R, editor. Advances in Child Development and Behavior. Amsterdam: Academic Press; 2002.
5.
Schutte NS, Malou JM, Hall LE, Haggerty DJ, Cooper JT, Golden CL, et al. Development and validation of the measure of emotional intelligence. Pers Individ Dif 1998;25:167-77.
6.
Mayer JD, Caruso DR, Salovey P. Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for intelligence. Intelligence 1999;27:267-98.
7.
Schutte NS, Malou JM, Thorsteinsson EB, Bhullar N, Rooke SE. Meta-anaylitic investigation of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Pers Individ Dif 2007;42:921-33.
8.
Jacobs M, Snow J, Geraci M, Vythilingam M, Blair RJ, Charney DS, et al. Association between level of emotional intelligence and severity of anxiety in generalized social phobia. J Anxiety Disord 2008;22:1487-95.
9.
Grooss JJ, Munoz RF. Emotion regulation and mental health. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 1995;2:151-64.
10.
Cloitre M. Sexual revictimization: Risk factors and prevention. In: Follette VM, Ruzek JI, Abueg FR, editors. Cognitive Behavioral Therapies For trauma. New York: Guilford Press; 1998. p. 278-304.
11.
Mennin DS, Heimberg RG, Turk CL, Fresco DM. Ap-plying an emotion regulation framework to integrative approaches to generalized anxiety disorder. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2002;9:85-90.
12.
Linehan MM. Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Borderline Personality Disorder. New York: The Guilford Press; 1993.
13.
Messman-Moore TL, Walsh KL, DiLillo D. Emotion dysregulation and risky sexual behavior in revictimization. Child Abuse Negl 2010;34:967-76.
14.
Banducci AN, Hofman EM, Lejuez CW, Koenen KC. The impact of childhood abuse on inpatient substance users: Specific links with risky sex, aggression, and emotion dysregulation. Child Abuse Negl 2014;38:928-38.
15.
Hayes SC, Wilson KG, Gifford EV, Follette VM, Strosahl K. Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: A functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996;64:1152-68.
16.
Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 2004;26:41-55.
17.
Sharp C, Pane H, Ha C, Venta A, Patel A, Sturek J, et al. Theory of mind and emotion regulation difficulties in adolescents with borderline traits. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2011;20:563-73.
18.
Mohammadloo S, Gharaee B, Lavasani F, Gohar MR. Investigating relationship between behavioral activation and behavioral inhibition emotion regulation difficult and metacognition with worry. J Res Cogn Behav Sci 2013;5:85-100.
19.
Tull MT, Weiss NH, Adams CE, Gratz K. The contribution of emotion regulation difficulties to risky sexual behavior within a sample of patients in residental substance abuse treatment. Addic Behav 2012;37:1048-92.
20.
Cole PM, Michel MK, Teti LO. The development of emotion regulation and dysregulation: A clinical perspective. In: Fox NA, editor. The Development of Emotion Regulation: Biological and Behavioral Considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Vol. 59. Serial No. 240. 1994. p. 73-100.
21.
Greenberg LS. Emotion-focused therapy: Coaching clients to work through their feelings. Washington, D.C: APA; 2002.
22.
Kring AM, Werner KH. Emotion regulation and psychopathology. In: Philippot P, Feldman RS, editors. The Regulation of Emotion. New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2004. p. 359-85.
23.
Khanzadeh M, Saidyan M, Hosseinchary M, Edrissi F. Factor structure and psychometric properties of difficulties in emotional regulation scale. J Behav Sci 2012;6:87-96.
24.
Orgeta V. Emotin dysregulation and anxiety in late adulthood. J Anxiety Disord 2011;25:1019-23.
25.
Fox HC, Hong KA, Sinha R. Difficulties in emotion regulation and impulse control in recently abstinent alcoholics compared with social drinkers. Addict Behav 2008;33:388-94.
26.
Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 2010;30:217-37.
27.
Evenden J. Impulsivity: A discussion of clinical and experimental findings. J Psychopharmacol 1999;13:180-92.
28.
Gratz KL. Risk factors for and functions of deliberate self-harm: An empirical and conceptual review. Clin Psychol Sci Pract 2003;10:192-205.
29.
Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. In: Fox NA, editor. The Development of Emotion Regulation: Biological and Behavioral Considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. Vol. 59. Serial No. 240. 1994. p. 25-52.
30.
Thompson RA, Calkins SD. The double-edged sword: Emotional regulation for children at risk. Dev Psychopathol 1996;8:163-82.
31.
Trinidad DR, Jahnsone CA. The association between emotional intelligence and early adolescent tobacco and alcohol use. Pers Indiv Dif 2000;32:95-105.
32.
Romer L, Orsillo SM, Salters-Pedneault K. Efficacy of an acceptance-based behavior therapy for generalied anxiety disorder: Evaluation in a randomized control trial. J Couns Clin Psychol 2008;76:1083-9.
33.
Mennin DS, Fresco DM. Emotion regulation as a framework for understanding and treating anxiety pathology. In: Kring AM, Slon DM, editors. Emotion Regulation in Psychopathology. New York: Guilford Press; 2009.
34.
Haviland MG, Hendryx MS, Shaw DG, Henry JP. Alexitymia in women and men hospitalized for psychoactive substance dependence. Compr Psychiatry 1994;35:124-8.

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None

Check


DOI: 10.4103/ATMPH.ATMPH_617_17

Tables

[Table 1], [Table 2]

Paul Mies has now been involved with test reports and comparing products for a decade. He is a highly sought-after specialist in these areas as well as in general health and nutrition advice. With this expertise and the team behind atmph.org, they test, compare and report on all sought-after products on the Internet around the topics of health, slimming, beauty and more. The results are ultimately summarized and disclosed to readers.

LEAVE A REPLY

Please enter your comment!
Please enter your name here